Anatomy of the human ear |
Micro-CT scan of a ball python's skull and ear. Red: mandible; dark blue: quadrate; green: columella; purple/light blue: inner ear chambers From Christensen et al. 2012 |
Diagram of the ear of a watersnake (Nerodia) Modified from Wever 1978 |
Hearing range of various animals, not including snakes |
Audibility curves for living reptiles, including birds (left). The lower the curve, the quieter a sound can be detected at a given frequency. You can see that snakes cannot hear very quiet sounds, but otherwise are not that much worse than other reptiles (although their hearing sucks compared to, say, owls). Note the different y-axes. From Dooling et al. 2000. |
1 This is necessary because "hearing" evolved under water. Many fishes and fully aquatic amphibians (such as amphiumas) have a network of hair-like cells all over their body, which is called a lateral line system. The lateral line allows them to sense water-borne vibrations using their entire body like one big eardrum. When early amniotes emerged onto land, the inner ear was still adapted to detecting fluid-borne vibrations, and the eardrum and outer ear evolved to facilitate collection of airborne sounds and translation of them into fluid-borne ones. These adaptations were further refined as amniotes began to hold their bodies off the ground (lizards, mammals) or fly (birds), minimizing their ability to pick up ground-borne vibrations with their ears. Snakes probably have a better capacity to pick up ground-borne vibrations than most amniotes, since at least some part of their body is in contact with the ground (or a tree) most of the time. To date, no one has examined hearing in fully aquatic snakes.↩
2 Many burrowing and aquatic amniotes have lost their external ear opening, because their need to detect airborne sounds is minimal, they can rely mostly on ground-borne vibrations, and their middle/inner ear could be damaged during burrowing or swimming if it was exposed. Amphisbaeneans and other lizards lacking external ears hear mostly ground-borne vibrations, which makes sense considering that many of them are fossorial and spend most of their lives with most of their bodies in contact with the ground. Amphisbaeneans have lost more of their airborne sound detection capacity than most burrowing lizards, in that, like snakes, they have also lost their tympanum and have their columella connected directly to their lower jaw (some naked mole rats have a similar jaw-middle ear connection and rely heavily on vibrational communication). One leading hypothesis suggests that snakes evolved from burrowing ancestors, and another suggests that they evolved from aquatic ancestors, so perhaps snakes lost and then regained an ability to hear airborne sounds. Other limbless squamates, such as pygopod geckos, specialize in making high-frequency vocalizations and have sensitive hearing to match.↩
3 At least two studies have investigated whether cobras can hear the music played by snake charmers, and concluded that cobras are responding to tactile and visual stimuli, not auditory.↩
REFERENCES
Clack. J.A. 1997. The evolution of tetrapod ears and the fossil record. Brain, Behavior, and Evolution 50:198-212 <link>
Dooling, R. J., Lohr, B., & Dent, M. L. 2000. Hearing in birds and reptiles. Pp. 308-359 in Comparative Hearing in Birds and Reptiles. Ed. by Robert J. Dooling, Richard R. Fay, and Arthur N. Popper. Springer New York <link>
Fuong, H., Keeley, K. N., Bulut, Y., & Blumstein, D. T. 2014. Heterospecific alarm call eavesdropping in nonvocal, white-bellied copper-striped skinks, Emoia cyanura. Animal Behaviour, 95:129-135 <link>
Hartline, PH. 1971. Physiological basis for detection of sound and vibration in snakes. Journal of Experimental Biology 54:349-371 <link>
Ito, R., & Mori, A. 2010. Vigilance against predators induced by eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls in a non-vocal lizard Oplurus cuvieri cuvieri (Reptilia: Iguania). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 277:1275-1280 <link>
Köppl, C., Manley, G. A., Popper, A. N., & Fay, R. R. 2014. Insights from Comparative Hearing Research. Springer New York <link>
Manley, G. A. 2012. Peripheral hearing mechanisms in reptiles and birds (Vol. 26). Springer Science & Business Media <link>
Manley, G. A., & Fay, R. R. (Eds.). 2013. Evolution of the Vertebrate Auditory System. Springer Science & Business Media <link>
Wever, E. G. 1978. The Reptile Ear: Its Structure and Function. Princeton: Princeton University Press <not available online>
Wever, EG and JA Vernon. 1960. The problem of hearing in snakes. Journal of Auditory Research 1:77-83 <not available online>
Young, B. A. 1997. A review of sound production and hearing in snakes, with a discussion of intraspecific acoustic communication in snakes. Journal of the Pennsylvania Academy of Science 71:39–46 <not available online>
Young, B. A., & Aguiar, A. 2002. Response of western diamondback rattlesnakes Crotalus atrox to airborne sounds. Journal of Experimental Biology 205:3087-3092 <link>
Young, B. A., & Morain, M. 2002. The use of ground-borne vibrations for prey localization in the Saharan sand vipers (Cerastes). Journal of Experimental Biology 205:661-665 <link>